Biodiversity Data Journal :
Research Article
|
Corresponding author: Kaviarasu Munian (kaviarasu@frim.gov.my), Nor Atiqah Norazlimi (atiqah@uthm.edu.my)
Academic editor: Caio J. Carlos
Received: 07 Nov 2024 | Accepted: 07 Jan 2025 | Published: 15 Jan 2025
© 2025 Nur Athirah Fauzi, Kaviarasu Munian, Nur Aina Amira Mahyudin, Nor Atiqah Norazlimi
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Fauzi NA, Munian K, Mahyudin NAA, Norazlimi NA (2025) Ecological insights on the feeding behaviour of waterbirds in an Important Bird and Biodiversity Area of South West Johor Coast, Malaysia. Biodiversity Data Journal 13: e141250. https://doi.org/10.3897/BDJ.13.e141250
|
|
Mangroves and mudflats are essential intertidal habitats that support benthic communities, providing critical feeding grounds for waterbirds. However, the degradation of these habitats due to coastal reclamation poses significant threats to prey availability and waterbird populations along the South est Johor Coast. While most avian research in Johor focuses on forest birds, studies on coastal waterbirds, particularly their feeding ecology, remain scarce. Understanding the feeding ecology of waterbirds is crucial for strengthening conservation efforts in vulnerable intertidal habitats. This study investigated the feeding behaviour and diet composition of waterbirds along the South West Johor Coast, Malaysia. Fieldwork was conducted in three coastal mudflats from November 2020 to May 2021, employing the direct observation technique. A total of 576 hours of observation were recorded, averaging 2 hours and 19 minutes per focal observation. Of 17 waterbird species recorded, only 11 species were included in the analysis based on sufficient data. The results revealed that waterbirds primarily consumed fish, molluscs, worms, crabs and unidentified prey, with fish comprising 25% of their overall diet. Feeding behaviour varied significantly by morphology traits, with larger waterbirds demonstrating higher feeding efficiency. Despite lower feeding rates and shorter feeding durations, larger species had a greater percentage of successful feeding attempts, indicating their superior ability to meet energy requirements. These findings provide crucial baseline data for understanding waterbird feeding ecology and highlight the importance of conserving the intertidal habitats. This research contributes to the development of targeted conservation strategies for waterbirds in the Important Bird and Biodiversity Area (IBA) of the South West Johor Coast, Malaysia, an area increasingly at risk from habitat degradation.
Important Bird and Biodiversity Area (IBA), feeding behaviour, intertidal, Scolopacidae, waterbirds
Intertidal mudflats have been valued for their ecosystem functions and services, serving as a foundation for conservation initiatives over the past decades (
Waterbirds are essential components of wetland ecosystems, utilising various habitats for nesting, breeding, foraging, stopover and wintering during migration (
In Malaysia, intertidal flats cover an estimated 52,951 hectares or roughly 1% of the nation’s total wetland area, with the majority located in the states of Selangor and Johor (
Apart from policies and laws, the national spatial planning of development incorporated coastal wetlands as one of the environmentally sensitive areas (ESAs) under the National Physical Plan (NPP). The NPP defines ESAs based on environmental, physical, cultural and climatic criteria and mandates that these areas remain undisturbed. Preservation or sustainable management of ESAs is required, guided by their type, sensitivity and ecological significance (
In this context, research on waterbirds in Malaysia has predominantly focused on non-coastal wetlands (
Notably, there is a significant gap in knowledge regarding the diets, foraging rates and prey availability for waterbirds in this region. Understanding the feeding ecology is intrinsically linked to population dynamics and provides insights into trophic interactions, such as prey selection, evolutionary adaptations, predation and energy transfer within and across ecosystems (
The study was focused on the mudflat areas along the IBA in the southern part of Peninsular Malaysia, namely South West Johor Coast. Three study sites have been chosen within two districts (Pontian and Muar), which are (i) Tanjung Piai National Park, (ii) Pontian Kechil coastal areas, and (iii) Pantai Leka and Pantai Mesra Seri Menanti, Muar (Fig.
Location and protection status of study sites. Pantai Leka (PL), Pantai Mesra Seri Menanti (PSM), Pontian Kechil (PK), Tanjung Piai (TP)
District |
Study sites |
Coordinates |
Protection status |
||
Latitude |
Longitude |
||||
Muar | Pantai Leka | PL 1 | Not protected | ||
Pantai Mesra Seri Menanti | PSM 1 | ||||
PSM 2 | |||||
Pontian | Pontian Kechil | PK 1 | Not protected | ||
PK 2 | |||||
PK 3 | |||||
Tanjung Piai | TP 1 | Legally protected | |||
TP 2 | |||||
TP 3 |
A standardised sampling protocol for each site was carried out with the same equipment and tools with the same sampling duration. A total of nine study plots, situated specifically on tidal flats of the three study sites, with three plots for each study site, were chosen for waterbird observations. The size of each observation plot is approximately 100 m in length and the width is according to the distance between the shore and the edge of water that ranged from 0 m to 700 m, as tidal flats are always affected by tidal cycles (
The observations were commenced from November 2020 until May 2021. At each site, three plots were established and two observation sessions were carried out for six days period. Surveys were alternated amongst the three plots within each site to ensure comprehensive spatial coverage. The observations were done during the Movement Control Order phase during the COVID-19 outbreak, which restricted the number of observers for the study to two personnel.
To minimise observer bias, the same two observers participated in all sessions, underwent standardised training in bird identification and data recording and resolved any discrepancies collaboratively, based on established field guides and protocols. The study focused on migratory phases, the southwards migration from June to December and the northwards migration occurring from February to May. Observations were conducted in four time intervals: 0800 to 1000 hours, 1000 to 1200 hours, 1400 to 1600 hours and 1600 to 1800 hours. The interval between 1200 and 1400 hours was excluded from the study due to the lack of waterbird activity, as determined through preliminary sampling. This systematic approach produced detailed insights into waterbird feeding behaviour across different migration periods.
The detailed feeding behaviour of waterbirds in three study sites was observed through focal observation with the aid of spotting scopes (Nikon 13-30 x 50 mm ED Angled). In addition, some of the observations were recorded using a DSLR Camera (Nikon D5500) with a Nikkor lens (Nikon 200-500 mm F/5.6E ED VR AF-S) when possible. A stopwatch was used for each focal observation to determine the time spent by waterbirds during feeding. The observation was conducted during the low tide period only. This reduces the bias in observation, as only large waterbirds can forage during high tide since they have longer legs. In each focal observation, an individual was selected from a flock. The selected waterbird needed to be actively feeding from the time the waterbird began actively searching for prey until the prey was completely swallowed. If the selected waterbird left the observation area in the span of 30 seconds, the observation was discarded from the analysis (
To avoid multiple observations of the same individual, the next selected waterbird needed to be located at least 10 m away from the previously observed individual. However, when there were more than one species of waterbird feeding simultaneously within the same observation area, the next waterbird for observation needed to be selected from a different species flock. The behavioural record was based on the method reported by
a) Pecks per minute: Pecks are defined as the feeding attempts when a waterbird is collecting prey from the surface of the substrate. These data were measured in this study to calculate the feeding rate and percentage of successful attempts by waterbird species. The feeding rate is the total number of feeding attempts (pecks or probes) made by waterbirds per minute. Meanwhile, the percentage of successful attempts was calculated by dividing the number of successful attempts that consumed the prey per minute by the feeding rate per minute and then multiplying the value by 100 to get the percentage. Prey items per minute were then used to determine the success rate. The success rate is the number of prey items consumed by each observed waterbird per minute. The summarised formula is as follows:
\(\text{Percentage of successful attempts (%)} = \left (\frac{\text{Number of successful attempts (per minute)}}{\text{Feeding rate (per minute)}} \right) \times 100\)
b) Prey types consumed: The types of prey were classified into fish, mollusc, worm, crab and unknown. Prey that cannot be seen clearly during observation were classified into the unknown group. This group of data was used to determine the diet composition of each waterbird species.
c) Time spent foraging: The time was measured starting from when the waterbird was actively searching for its prey until the prey was swallowed entirely (estimates were recorded in minutes).
d) Flocking behaviour: The waterbirds were classified as solitary if they were observed foraging alone or separate from other waterbirds. In contrast, the waterbirds were classified as an intraspecific flock if they were foraging in a group or flock of the same species, while the waterbirds foraging in the mixed-species flock were classified as an interspecific flock.
e) Foraging techniques: The foraging techniques engaged by waterbirds were classified into three types:
All the data collected were organised using the Microsoft Excel software. For the analysis, Paleontological Statistic (PAST) (
The average time spent feeding for each species was calculated by dividing the total time spent feeding by each species by the total number of individuals observed for each species. The differences in foraging behaviour between species and behavioural categories were compared using the Kruskal Wallis test because the data collected were not normal. The non-parametric multiple comparison test was used when the Kruskal-Wallis test indicated a significant difference.
In addition, the Spearman Correlation test was conducted to test the relationship between time spent feeding by the waterbirds and their success rate, followed by the relationship between feeding rate and success rate of waterbirds. The Spearman Correlation test measures the strength and direction of association between two variables. The test produce an R-value ranging from +1 to -1. R-value of +1 indicates a perfect positive association, while the value of 0 indicates no association and -1 indicates a perfect negative association. The closer the R-value is to 0, the weaker the association between the two variables.
The total observation hours were 576 hours, with an average of 2 hours and 19 minutes spent for each focal observation for 11 species. According to a previous study by
List of waterbird species observed. Partially Migrant (PM), Resident (R), Migrant (M), Vagrant (V), Least Concern (LC), Near Threatened (NT), Vulnerable (VU), Endangered (EN).
Family |
Species |
Common Name |
Distribution |
IUCN Status (2024) |
No. of focal observation |
Ardeidae |
Ardea cinerea |
Grey Heron |
PM |
LC |
35 |
Egretta garzetta |
Little Egret |
PM |
LC |
38 |
|
Ardea alba |
Great Egret |
PM |
LC |
30 |
|
Butorides striata |
Striated Heron |
PM |
LC |
35 |
|
Charadriidae |
Anarhynchus atrifrons |
Tibetan Sand-plover |
M |
EN |
40 |
Scolopacidae |
Tringa totanus |
Common Redshank |
M |
LC |
67 |
Numenius phaeopus |
Whimbrel |
M |
LC |
12 |
|
Xenus cinereus |
Terek Sandpiper |
M |
LC |
12 |
|
Actitis hypoleucos |
Common Sandpiper |
M |
LC |
13 |
|
Ciconiidae |
Leptoptilos javanicus |
Lesser Adjutant |
R |
VU |
32 |
Mycteria cineria |
Milky Stork |
R |
EN |
14 |
|
Total |
328 |
In terms of diet composition, five prey groups, namely fish, mollusc, worms, crabs and unknown groups, were observed as the main diet for waterbirds. Based on the observation, the fish group was the most preferred diet amongst all the waterbird species, accounting for a total of 25% of the waterbirds’ diet, followed by worms (23%), unknown (22%), mollusc (19%) and crab (11%). The prey items categorised under the unknown group were the prey that could not be seen and identified during the observations.
All the observed species consumed fish, except for Common Sandpiper (Actitis hypoleucos), Terek Sandpiper (Xenus cinereus) and Tibetan Sand-plover (Anarhynchus atrifrons). Fish were also determined to be the main diet for large waterbirds, such as Milky Stork (Mycteria cineria), Lesser Adjutant (Leptoptilos javanicus), Little Egret (Egretta garzetta), Great Egret (Ardea alba) and Grey Heron (Ardea cinerea). Meanwhile, worms were mainly consumed by Common Redshank (Tringa totanus), Common Sandpiper (Actitis hypoleucos), Tibetan Sand-plover (Anarhynchus atrifrons) and Striated Heron (Butorides striata). Worms and mollusc were observed in the diet of all the waterbird species, except for Milky Stork, with Common Redshank consuming them most frequently (Fig.
In this study, waterbird species were observed to engage with different feeding techniques. However, there was a similar pattern in the preferred feeding techniques amongst the waterbirds with the same body size. Three feeding techniques were observed to be practised by waterbird species. All the individuals of the Striated Heron were practising the pause-travel technique, while the Whimbrel, Terek Sandpiper and Common Sandpiper were observed to practise the tactile-hunting technique solely. Grey Heron and Great Egret preferred to practise the pause-travel technique, although a small population of the individuals used the visual-feeding technique. In contrast, Little Egret, Lesser Adjutant, Common Redshank and Tibetan Sand-plover were seen to engage in both pause-travel and tactile hunting techniques. However, the pause-travel technique was the most preferred by Little Egret and Lesser Adjutant, while the Common Redshank and Tibetan Sand-plover opted more for the tactile-hunting technique. Only Milky Stork was observed to practise all three feeding techniques equally. There was a significant difference between the time spent on feeding and the feeding techniques engaged by the waterbirds (H = 5.665, p = 0.047). The pairwise comparison test showed that the differences lie between the visual-feeding technique and the pause-travel technique (p = 0.045), as well as between the visual-feeding and tactile-hunting techniques (p = 0.024) (Table
Sample size (n), mean and standard error of time spent on feeding and foraging techniques used by waterbirds.
Species |
Foraging techniques |
Time spent feeding (min) |
||
n |
Mean |
Standard error |
||
Grey Heron |
Pause-Travel |
26 |
7.96 |
1.64 |
Tactile Hunting |
0 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
|
Visual Feeding |
9 |
6.53 |
2.60 |
|
Great Egret |
Pause-Travel |
26 |
8.01 |
1.67 |
Tactile Hunting |
0 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
|
Visual Feeding |
4 |
6.15 |
3.59 |
|
Striated Heron |
Pause-Travel |
35 |
11.98 |
2.24 |
Tactile Hunting |
0 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
|
Visual Feeding |
0 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
|
Little Egret |
Pause-Travel |
35 |
7.3 |
1.30 |
Tactile Hunting |
3 |
7.07 |
5.14 |
|
Visual Feeding |
0 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
|
Lesser Adjutant |
Pause-Travel |
28 |
8.72 |
1.69 |
Tactile Hunting |
4 |
6.59 |
3.82 |
|
Visual Feeding |
0 |
0 |
0.00 |
|
Milky Stork |
Pause-Travel |
4 |
11.86 |
7.03 |
Tactile Hunting |
5 |
11.88 |
6.99 |
|
Visual Feeding |
5 |
12.99 |
6.81 |
|
Tibetan Sand-plover |
Pause-Travel |
2 |
6.415 |
6.51 |
Tactile Hunting |
38 |
11.88 |
6.99 |
|
Visual Feeding |
0 |
0 |
0.00 |
|
Whimbrel |
Pause-Travel |
0 |
0 |
0.00 |
Tactile Hunting |
12 |
8.31 |
2.68 |
|
Visual Feeding |
0 |
0 |
0.00 |
|
Terek Sandpiper |
Pause-Travel |
0 |
0 |
0.00 |
Tactile Hunting |
12 |
8.4 |
2.60 |
|
Visual Feeding |
0 |
0 |
0.00 |
|
Common Sandpiper |
Pause-Travel |
0 |
0 |
0.00 |
Tactile Hunting |
13 |
8.03 |
2.39 |
|
Visual Feeding |
0 |
0 |
0.00 |
|
Common Redshank |
Pause-Travel |
5 |
7.09 |
3.58 |
Tactile Hunting |
62 |
9.32 |
1.32 |
|
Visual Feeding |
0 |
0 |
0.00 |
The time spent feeding for each waterbird individual was recorded and the average minutes spent during feeding were calculated (Fig.
Although most waterbird species exhibit a distinct flocking behaviour, observational data show that 90% of Great Egrets and 88% of Grey Herons were recorded feeding individually (solitarily). In contrast, the rest of the individuals were observed in a group of the same species, also known as intraspecific flocks (Fig.
Feeding success is crucial for waterbirds, mainly maintaining their body fitness and fuelling their energy supply before the long-distance migration and breeding activities (
Based on these observations, Common Redshank and Tibetan Sand-plover had the highest feeding rates compared to other species (Table
Values of feeding rate, success rate and percentage of successful attempts between waterbirds.
Species |
Feeding Rate (pecks/probes) |
Success rate |
Percentage of successful attempts (%) |
||
n |
Mean |
SE |
|||
Grey Heron |
35 |
5.16 |
0.751 |
3.57 |
89 |
Great Egret |
30 |
4.32 |
0.618 |
3.1 |
70 |
Striated Heron |
35 |
1.21 |
0.324 |
1.37 |
90 |
Little Egret |
38 |
6.32 |
0.972 |
3.31 |
64 |
Lesser Adjutant |
32 |
5.22 |
0.859 |
4.1 |
89 |
Milky Stork |
14 |
5.16 |
0.836 |
3.3 |
73 |
Tibetan Sand-plover |
40 |
12.36 |
1.641 |
8.1 |
67 |
Whimbrel |
12 |
8.41 |
1.29 |
5.53 |
75 |
Terek Sandpiper |
12 |
11.08 |
1.614 |
8.26 |
64 |
Common Sandpiper |
13 |
11.57 |
1.637 |
8.0 |
65 |
Common Redshank |
67 |
13.65 |
1.835 |
9.9 |
78 |
A significant difference was observed in success rate between species by Kruskal Wallis test (H = 2.87, p = 0.046). Common Redshank recorded the highest success rate and measured the highest feeding rate. Spearman correlation analysis was conducted to test the relationship between feeding and success rates (R = 0.927, p < 0.05). The R-value was close to 1, thus indicating a strong positive correlation since the feeding rate increases with the success rate (Fig.
The findings of this study lies in combining both direct field observations of species-specific behaviors (e.g., Terek Sandpipers and Whimbrels handling large crabs, Striated Herons’ pause-travel tactics) with insights into how morphological differences influence feeding success. By highlighting these connections, our work addresses a critical gap in previous research, where emphasis often focused on abundance and distribution without fully exploring how anatomical and behavioral adaptations determine foraging efficiency. Understanding these nuances is essential for interpreting how different species meet their energetic needs within intertidal environments and can inform broader conservation strategies aimed at maintaining functional diversity in waterbird populations. Moreover, these individual foraging adaptations intersect with social behaviors, such as flocking, which can further modify feeding success and predator avoidance.
Feeding behaviour and prey selection by waterbirds are heavily influenced by their morphology, particularly the morphology of bill length and shape (
On the other hand, although Whimbrel and Terek Sandpiper can be considered small-bodied waterbirds, both species have a long, unique-shaped bill that curves upward or downward. The special bill structure allows them to catch larger prey than small-bodied waterbirds with shorter bills. Large crabs are a major prey item in the diet of Whimbrel and Terek Sandpiper species observed in the overall study sites.
There are debates that still remain on how smaller-bodied waterbird species, such as the Terek Sandpiper, manage to consume large crabs, given their relatively narrow, curved bills. While both Whimbrels and Terek Sandpipers possess long bills suitable for extracting crabs from sediments, it might seem implausible for them to swallow a large crab wholly.
Herons, egrets and storks possess very long and large bills that allow them to consume fish as their main diet. Their bill structure results in a stronger grip on the prey, especially for fish, since fish can produce a powerful jerk. These species are commonly observed to forage at the edge of receding water during the low tide, where the fish are usually abundant. However, in Tanjung Piai, due to the presence of reclamation land close to the shore, the mudflat area is fully exposed during the peak period of the ebbing tide. As a result, the large waterbird species needed to find alternative prey during those periods, and these species were seen consuming other preys, including crabs, molluscs, and worms. Larger-billed waterbirds presumably have access to a broader range of prey resources, including deeper-buried invertebrates (
Large-bodied waterbirds, egrets, and herons preferred to practice pause-travel techniques, while tactile-hunting techniques were preferred by small-bodied waterbirds, including Tibetan Sand-plover, Whimbrel, Terek Sandpiper, Common Sandpiper and Common Redshank. Large waterbirds feed using less active methods compared to smaller waterbirds, which use more dynamic techniques (
According to
Additionally, Striated Herons demonstrated a pause-travel foraging technique, scanning prey visually from a stationary position before capturing it. This strategy likely increases their success rate by reducing the chance of startling potential prey, but it also extends the total feeding time required to locate suitable items. These findings align with earlier work (
Our observations also suggest that large waterbirds can consume fewer, larger prey items with high profitability, thereby fulfilling their energetic needs in less time. This reduction in foraging effort contrasts with the behavior of smaller waterbirds, which often forage continuously for smaller prey containing lower caloric content. Previous studies (
Waterbirds often achieve greater foraging success by feeding in flocks, and two primary hypotheses have been proposed to explain this adaptive behavior. First, group foraging enhances predator avoidance and reduces vigilance costs (
Competition for resources generally arises through depletion (consuming the available prey) or interference (
By contrast, large waterbirds, such as Milky Storks and Egrets, invest less total time in active foraging yet achieve high percentages of successful attempts. Their ability to capture and handle larger prey items stems from their larger bills and overall body size (
In conclusion, the feeding rate, success rate and percentage of successful attempts of waterbirds in this study are highly influenced by the differences in waterbirds’ morphological traits, feeding techniques engaged and flocking behaviour. All of these are closely related to their time spent on feeding. Large waterbirds recorded a higher percentage of successful attempts, even though lower feeding rates and shorter feeding times. All this evidence pointed out that larger waterbirds are more efficient at feeding and fulfilling their energy requirement. By understanding the feeding efficiency and adaptability of different waterbird species, the conservationists and managers can better anticipate which species are at higher risk and which habitats are most critical to preserve for future.
This project was funded by the Ministry of Natural Resources and Environmental Sustainablity under 12th Malaysia Plan (P23085100210003), Tier 1 Grant Research (Q509) and UTHM GPPS Grant (H649). This research was conducted under PERHILITAN research permit JPHL/TN (IP): 100-34/1.24 Jld 19 (3)-W002761522 and Perbadanan Taman Negara Johor research permit TNJ 700-2/5/1: Penyelidikan/INV/2020/00057. A special thank to Yeap Chin Aik for contributing map of IBA South West Johor Coast. We hereby acknowledge NRES, FRIM, UTHM, Department of Wildlife and National Park (PERHILITAN) and Perbadanan Taman Negara Johor (PTNJ) for providing the necessary funding, facilities and assistance.
N.A.F and N.A.N conceived the original idea and contributed to the design of the study. N.A.F., N.A.A.M and K.M gathered the data, performed the analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. K.M. and N.A.A.M aided with the interpretation of the results and provided critical feedback on the manuscript. K.M., N.A.A.M., N.A.F. and N.A.A.M discussed and agreed on the final draft of the manuscript.